The New York Times
yet again features
odd pronouncements on
bisexuality by J. Michael Bailey:
A trans news update by Lynn Conway
April 10, 2007:
You would have thought that the New York Times had learned its lesson when it
promoted J. Michael Bailey views on bisexuality back in July 2005. In an article
entitled "Straight,
Gay or Lying? Bisexuality Revisited" by Benedict Carey, J. Michael Bailey
attacked the identities of bisexual men, claiming that bisexual men are "lying" and that most are just gay men after all.
He based his
pronouncements on a small-sample-set study of sexual arousals to pornography,
using a disputed device known as a
plethysmograph
to measure such arousals.
That earlier Times article raised a storm of protest against Bailey's
pseudoscientific claims, including alerts and fact sheets sent out by such
respected groups as
GLAAD, FAIR and the
NGLTF (see
background references below). All those groups pointed out Bailey's checkered
past and lack of credibility (see the
NGLTF's
Fact Sheet in particular), and were later assured by the Times' Editors that
future sex science reporting would be more careful in calibrating their sources.
However, it seems that the Times Editors either hadn't learned anything from
past experience with Mr. Bailey or were disingenuous when handling the
complaints from the gay community about the earlier article. For yet again, on April 10, 2007,
the Times gave Mr. Bailey center stage in
national media, referring to him as "an expert on sexual orientation" and
featuring the following pronouncements in a sex science
article entitled "Pas de Deux of Sexuality Is Written in the Genes":
“If you can’t make a male
attracted to other males by cutting off his penis, how strong could any
psychosocial effect be?” said J. Michael Bailey, an expert on sexual orientation
at
Northwestern University.
Presumably the masculinization
of the brain shapes some neural circuit that makes women desirable. If so, this
circuitry is wired differently in gay men. In experiments in which subjects are
shown photographs of desirable men or women, straight men are aroused by women,
gay men by men.
Such experiments do not show the
same clear divide with women. Whether women describe themselves as straight or
lesbian, “Their sexual arousal seems to be relatively indiscriminate — they get
aroused by both male and female images,” Dr. Bailey said. “I’m not even sure
females have a sexual orientation. But they have sexual preferences. Women are
very picky, and most choose to have sex with men.” - (New
York Times, 4-10-07)
In this new article the Times only
implicitly presents Bailey's "discovery" that male bisexuality does not
exist, and Bailey restrains himself from making accusations that
bisexual men are "liars" - accusations he's made so often in the past.
Nevertheless, we see Bailey pressing on in his efforts to erase male bisexuality - and
we see the New York Times continuing to provide him with a stage on which to do
that.
Even stranger is the Times'
publication of Mr. Bailey's pronouncement as "an expert on sexual orientation"
that he's "not even sure females have a sexual orientation". Many readers
found this statement to be quite over the top. For example, the
Editors at Scientific American responded to Bailey's odd
statement that same day, calling it a "zinger" and discussing it as follows in an
Editors Blog entry entitled "Do women have a sexual orientation":
Today the Times ran a
thorough and initially unremarkable account
of the genetic and biological underpinnings of sex and sexuality, pretty tame
stuff, right up until this zinger:
Presumably the masculinization of the brain shapes some neural circuit that
makes women desirable. If so, this circuitry is wired differently in gay men. In
experiments in which subjects are shown photographs of desirable men or women,
straight men are aroused by women, gay men by men.
Such experiments do not show the same clear divide
with women. Whether women describe themselves as straight or lesbian, "Their
sexual arousal seems to be relatively indiscriminate - they get aroused by both
male and female images," Dr. Bailey said. "I'm not even sure females have a
sexual orientation. But they have sexual preferences. Women are very picky, and
most choose to have sex with men."
It's clear that
Dr. Bailey (who is something of a
controversial figure in his field) isn't much of a
social constructionist (as that term
applies to gender). But I'm curious how he can completely discount the influence
of society's sanctioning of some types of behaviors and not others on the
outcome of the experiments in question--which are, I'm supposing, tests of
arousal in the presence of provocative images of either men or women, conducted
on adult human beings who have already experienced a lifetime of cultural
indoctrination.
Later in the piece, in
a passage that seems to muddy the waters more than it boosts Dr. Bailey's
hypothesis, we get:
Sexual orientation, at least for men, seems to be settled before birth. "I think
most of the scientists working on these questions are convinced that the
antecedents of sexual orientation in males are happening early in life, probably
before birth," Dr. Breedlove said, "whereas for females, some are probably born
to become gay, but clearly some get there quite late in life."
Leaving aside women
for a moment, whose gender identities are no doubt every bit as complicated to
construct as those of men, when we consider that the construction of the male
gender is dependent on quite a few developmental events, any one of which can
turn out differently than it usually does and lead to an alternate gender
identity (e.g., chromosomal abnormalities, problems at the onset of puberty that
result from a malfunction of hormonal systems, masculinization / feminization of
the brain in the womb due to maternal effects) I wonder how well (in the absence
of controlled experiments on humans which, thankfully, aren't allowed in civil
society) we can make claims like "women have a sexual preference, but only men
have a sexual orientation."
(And that's all before
we get to the even-more-endlessly-debatable questions that arise when we start
to talk about culture's influence on gender.) - (Scientific
American.com, 4-10-07)
Could it be that the New York Times
lets Mr. Bailey run off at the mouth in their articles, knowing full
well the strangeness of his thinking - and perhaps counting on him to make such articles
controversial? Do "controversial" and oddly-bizarre sex
science articles boost circulation, by encouraging impulse-buying at newsstands?
Whatever the reason for the New York Times' ongoing display of Mr. Bailey's
sex thinking, they aren't really doing him any
favors. As time goes by and his statements become ever stranger, he's
becoming a laughing stock and embarrassment not only for himself but for the
entire field of academic psychology - as seen in the Scientific American Editors blog
and further discussions in The Stranger.
Lynn Conway
References:
4-10-07: The
New York Times, "Pas de Deux of Sexuality Is Written in the Genes", by
Nicholas Wade
4-10-07: "Do
women have a sexual orientation?", by Christopher Mims; Scientific American,
.
See also the
following commentaries:
"Women: We Have No Sexual Orientation", by Jen Graves; The
Stranger SLOG (Seattle), April 10, 2007.
"Re: Women: We Have No Sexual Orientation", by Erica C. Barnett;
The Stranger SLOG (Seattle), April 10, 2007.
Background on the
New York Times' past promotion of Bailey's pronouncements on
bisexuality:
8-22-05: American Sexuality Magazine, Vol. 3, No. 4:
"Sexual Prejudice: The erasure of bisexuals in academia and the
media," by Loraine Hutchins.
(PDF)
7-20-05: AMERICAblog.com: "NYT Science Section: Another
Botched Story", by Michael in New York
7-19-05: A TIMES Opinion: "The folly of putting labels on
sexuality: Straight, gay or bisexual attraction is far too nuanced
and subjective to withstand crude statistical analysis."
7-15-05: Washington Blade: "Report on bisexuality study angers
gay activists: Critics say New York Times ignored methodology,
author’s past", by Elizabeth Weill-Greenberg. (see
also New York Blade, 7-15-05)
7-11-05: NGLTF Press Release: "National Gay and Lesbian Task
Force denounces New York Times story..." (Fact
Sheet, PDF)
7-08-05:
FAIR Action Alert: New York Times
Suggests Bisexuals Are "Lying".
Paper fails to disclose study author's controversial history.
7-07-05: QueerDay.com: Bisexuality research the work of
crackpot J. Michael Bailey.
7-07-05: GLAAD "Write Now!" Alert: 'NEW YORK TIMES' PROMOTES
BISEXUAL STEREOTYPES..."
http://www.nytimes.com/2007/04/10/health/10gene.html?pagewanted=1&ei=5087%0A&em&en=b404a6db2fd69c6f&ex=1176350400
The New York Times
April 10, 2007
Pas de Deux
of Sexuality Is Written in the Genes
By
Nicholas Wade
When it comes to
the matter of desire, evolution leaves little to chance. Human sexual behavior
is not a free-form performance, biologists are finding, but is guided at every
turn by genetic programs.
Desire between
the sexes is not a matter of choice. Straight men, it seems, have neural
circuits that prompt them to seek out women; gay men have those prompting them
to seek other men. Women’s brains may be organized to select men who seem likely
to provide for them and their children. The deal is sealed with other neural
programs that induce a burst of romantic love, followed by long-term attachment.
So much fuss, so
intricate a dance, all to achieve success on the simple scale that is all
evolution cares about, that of raisingthe greatest number of children to
adulthood. Desire may seem the core of human sexual behavior, but it is just the
central act in a long drama whose script is written quite substantially in the
genes.
In the womb, the
body of a developing fetus is female by default and becomes male if the
male-determining gene known as SRY is present. This dominant gene, the Y
chromosome’s proudest and almost only possession, sidetracks the reproductive
tissue from its ovarian fate and switches it into becoming testes.
Hormones from the testes, chiefly
testosterone, mold the body into male form.
In puberty, the
reproductive systems are primed for action by the brain. Amazing electrical
machine that it may be, the brain can also behave like a humble gland. In the
hypothalamus, at the central base of the brain, lie a cluster of about 2,000
neurons that ignite puberty when they start to secrete pulses of gonadotropin-releasing
hormone, which sets off a cascade of other hormones.
The trigger that
stirs these neurons is still unknown, but probably the brain monitors internal
signals as to whether the body is ready to reproduce and external cues as to
whether circumstances are propitious for yielding to desire.
Several advances
in the last decade have underlined the bizarre fact that the brain is a
full-fledged sexual organ, in that the two sexes have profoundly different
versions of it. This is the handiwork of testosterone, which masculinizes the
brain as thoroughly as it does the rest of the body.
It is a
misconception that the differences between men’s and women’s brains are small or
erratic or found only in a few extreme cases, Dr. Larry Cahill of the
University of California, Irvine, wrote last year in Nature Reviews
Neuroscience. Widespread regions of the cortex, the brain’s outer layer that
performs much of its higher-level processing, are thicker in women. The
hippocampus, where initial memories are formed, occupies a larger fraction of
the female brain.
Techniques for
imaging the brain have begun to show that men and women use their brains in
different ways even when doing the same thing. In the case of the amygdala, a
pair of organs that helps prioritize memories according to their emotional
strength, women use the left amygdala for this purpose but men tend to use the
right.
It is no
surprise that the male and female versions of the human brain operate in
distinct patterns, despite the heavy influence of culture. The male brain is
sexually oriented toward women as an object of desire. The most direct evidence
comes from a handful of cases, some of them circumcision accidents, in which boy
babies have lost their penises and been reared as female. Despite every social
inducement to the opposite, they grow up desiring women as partners, not men.
“If you can’t make a male
attracted to other males by cutting off his penis, how strong could any
psychosocial effect be?” said J. Michael Bailey, an expert on sexual orientation
at
Northwestern University.
Presumably the masculinization
of the brain shapes some neural circuit that makes women desirable. If so, this
circuitry is wired differently in gay men. In experiments in which subjects are
shown photographs of desirable men or women, straight men are aroused by women,
gay men by men.
Such experiments do not show
the same clear divide with women. Whether women describe themselves as straight
or lesbian, “Their sexual arousal seems to be relatively indiscriminate — they
get aroused by both male and female images,” Dr. Bailey said. “I’m not even sure
females have a sexual orientation. But they have sexual preferences. Women are
very picky, and most choose to have sex with men.”
Dr. Bailey believes that the
systems for sexual orientation and arousal make men go out and find people to
have sex with, whereas women are more focused on accepting or rejecting those
who seek sex with them.
Similar
differences between the sexes are seen by Marc Breedlove, a neuroscientist at
Michigan State University. “Most males are quite stubborn in their ideas
about which sex they want to pursue, while women seem more flexible,” he said.
Sexual
orientation, at least for men, seems to be settled before birth. “I think most
of the scientists working on these questions are convinced that the antecedents
of sexual orientation in males are happening early in life, probably before
birth,” Dr. Breedlove said, “whereas for females, some are probably born to
become gay, but clearly some get there quite late in life.”
Sexual behavior
includes a lot more than sex. Helen Fisher, an anthropologist at
Rutgers University, argues that three primary brain systems have evolved to
direct reproductive behavior. One is the sex drive that motivates people to seek
partners. A second is a program for romantic attraction that makes people fixate
on specific partners. Third is a mechanism for long-term attachment that induces
people to stay together long enough to complete their parental duties.
Romantic love,
which in its intense early stage “can last 12-18 months,” is a universal human
phenomenon, Dr. Fisher wrote last year in The Proceedings of the Royal Society,
and is likely to be a built-in feature of the brain. Brain imaging studies show
that a particular area of the brain, one associated with the reward system, is
activated when subjects contemplate a photo of their lover.
The best
evidence for a long-term attachment process in mammals comes from studies of
voles, a small mouselike rodent. A hormone called vasopressin, which is active
in the brain, leads some voles to stay pair-bonded for life. People possess the
same hormone, suggesting a similar mechanism could be at work in humans, though
this has yet to be proved.
Researchers have
devoted considerable effort to understanding homosexuality in men and women,
both for its intrinsic interest and for the light it could shed on the more
usual channels of desire. Studies of twins show that homosexuality, especially
among men, is quite heritable, meaning there is a genetic component to it. But
since gay men have about one-fifth as many children as straight men, any gene
favoring homosexuality should quickly disappear from the population.
Such genes could
be retained if gay men were unusually effective protectors of their nephews and
nieces, helping genes just like theirs get into future generations. But gay men
make no better uncles than straight men, according to a study by Dr. Bailey. So
that leaves the possibility that being gay is a byproduct of a gene that
persists because it enhances
fertility in other family members. Some studies have found that gay men have
more relatives than straight men, particularly on their mother’s side.
But Dr. Bailey
believes the effect, if real, would be more clear-cut. “Male homosexuality is
evolutionarily maladaptive,” he said, noting that the phrase means only that
genes favoring homosexuality cannot be favored by evolution if fewer such genes
reach the next generation.
A somewhat more
straightforward clue to the origin of homosexuality is the fraternal birth order
effect. Two Canadian researchers, Ray Blanchard and Anthony F. Bogaert, have
shown that having older brothers substantially increases the chances that a man
will be gay. Older sisters don’t count, nor does it matter whether the brothers
are in the house when the boy is reared.
The finding
suggests that male homosexuality in these cases is caused by some event in the
womb, such as “a maternal immune response to succeeding male pregnancies,” Dr.
Bogaert wrote last year in the
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. Antimale antibodies could
perhaps interfere with the usual masculinization of the brain that occurs before
birth, though no such antibodies have yet been detected.
The fraternal
birth order effect is quite substantial. Some 15 percent of gay men can
attribute their homosexuality to it, based on the assumption that 1 percent to 4
percent of men are gay, and each additional older brother increases the odds of
same-sex attraction by 33 percent.
The effect
supports the idea that the levels of circulating testosterone before birth are
critical in determining sexual orientation. But testosterone in the fetus cannot
be measured, and as adults, gay and straight men have the same levels of the
hormone, giving no clue to prenatal exposure. So the hypothesis, though
plausible, has not been proved.
A significant
recent advance in understanding the basis of sexuality and desire has been the
discovery that genes may have a direct effect on the sexual differentiation of
the brain. Researchers had long assumed that
steroid hormones like testosterone and
estrogen did all the heavy lifting of shaping the male and female brains.
But Arthur Arnold of the University of California, Los Angeles, has found that
male and female neurons behave somewhat differently when kept in laboratory
glassware. And last year Eric Vilain, also of U.C.L.A., made the surprising
finding that the SRY gene is active in certain cells of the brain, at least in
mice. Its brain role is quite different from its testosterone-related
activities, and women’s neurons presumably perform that role by other means.
It so happens
that an unusually large number of brain-related genes are situated on the X
chromosome. The sudden emergence of the X and Y chromosomes in brain function
has caught the attention of evolutionary biologists. Since men have only one X
chromosome, natural selection can speedily promote any advantageous mutation
that arises in one of the X’s genes. So if those picky women should be looking
for smartness in prospective male partners, that might explain why so many
brain-related genes ended up on the X.
“It’s popular
among male academics to say that females preferred smarter guys,” Dr. Arnold
said. “Such genes will be quickly selected in males because new beneficial
mutations will be quickly apparent.”
Several profound
consequences follow from the fact that men have only one copy of the many
X-related brain genes and women two. One is that many neurological diseases are
more common in men because women are unlikely to suffer mutations in both copies
of a gene.
Another is that
men, as a group, “will have more variable brain phenotypes,” Dr. Arnold writes,
because women’s second copy of every gene dampens the effects of mutations that
arise in the other.
Greater male
variance means that although average IQ is identical in men and women, there are
fewer average men and more at both extremes. Women’s care in selecting mates,
combined with the fast selection made possible by men’s lack of backup copies of
X-related genes, may have driven the divergence between male and female brains.
The same factors could explain, some researchers believe, why the human brain
has tripled in volume over just the last 2.5 million years.
Who can doubt
it? It is indeed desire that makes the world go round.
http://blog.sciam.com/index.php?title=do_women_have_a_sexual_orientation&more=1&c=1&tb=1&pb=1
BLOG: SCIAM OBSERVATIONS
Opinions,
arguments and analyses from the editors of Scientific
American
April 10, 2007
Do women have a sexual orientation?
by Christopher
Mims
Today the Times ran a
thorough and initially unremarkable account of the genetic and biological
underpinnings of sex and sexuality, pretty tame stuff, right up until this
zinger:
Presumably the
masculinization of the brain shapes some neural circuit that makes women
desirable. If so, this circuitry is wired differently in gay men. In experiments
in which subjects are shown photographs of desirable men or women, straight men
are aroused by women, gay men by men.
Such experiments
do not show the same clear divide with women. Whether women describe themselves
as straight or lesbian, "Their sexual arousal seems to be relatively
indiscriminate - they get aroused by both male and female images," Dr. Bailey
said. "I'm not even sure females have a sexual orientation. But they have sexual
preferences. Women are very picky, and most choose to have sex with men."
It's clear that
Dr. Bailey (who is something of a controversial figure in his field) isn't
much of a
social constructionist (as that term applies to gender). But I'm curious how
he can completely discount the influence of society's sanctioning of some types
of behaviors and not others on the outcome of the experiments in question--which
are, I'm supposing, tests of arousal in the presence of provocative images of
either men or women, conducted on adult human beings who have already
experienced a lifetime of cultural indoctrination.
Later in the piece, in a passage that seems to
muddy the waters more than it boosts Dr. Bailey's hypothesis, we get:
Sexual orientation,
at least for men, seems to be settled before birth. "I think most of the
scientists working on these questions are convinced that the antecedents of
sexual orientation in males are happening early in life, probably before birth,"
Dr. Breedlove said, "whereas for females, some are probably born to become gay,
but clearly some get there quite late in life."
Leaving aside women for a moment, whose gender
identities are no doubt every bit as complicated to construct as those of men,
when we consider that the construction of the male gender is dependent on quite
a few developmental events, any one of which can turn out differently than it
usually does and lead to an alternate gender identity (e.g., chromosomal
abnormalities, problems at the onset of puberty that result from a malfunction
of hormonal systems, masculinization / feminization of the brain in the womb due
to maternal effects) I wonder how well (in the absence of controlled experiments
on humans which, thankfully, aren't allowed in civil society) we can make claims
like "women have a sexual preference, but only men have a sexual orientation."
(And that's all before we get to the
even-more-endlessly-debatable questions that arise when we start to talk about
culture's influence on gender.)
Update:
The Stranger has a couple of other interesting takes on the Times article
that take the discussion in directions I hadn't even thought of - like the
overall (nascent) state of this kind of research.
Posted by
Christopher Mims
See also the following
commentaries on Bailey's pronouncements:
"Women: We Have No Sexual Orientation", by Jen Graves; The Stranger SLOG
(Seattle), April 10, 2007.
"Re: Women: We Have No Sexual Orientation", by Erica C. Barnett; The Stranger
SLOG (Seattle), April 10, 2007.
LynnConway.com >
Trans News Updates > Bailey
pronouncements on bisexuality in New York Times again