Dr. Peter Hegarty, University
of Surrey,
Dr Penny Lenihan,
University College London
Dr. Meg Barker, Middlesex University
Dr. Lyndsey Moon,
University of Newcastle
*A response to
the essay "Psychology
Perverted" - by
Dr. Joan
Roughgarden of Stanford University
First
published on UKPFC News
March 19, 2004
"We are
particularly concerned that Bailey's work will be seen as representative
of scientific psychological research,
both by the
trans community and by other sections of the public."
"...the
danger that Bailey's expressed anti-trans opinions might be confused
with scientific evidence
is
particularly high in this case."
As a social psychologist (PH), a consultant counselling psychologist
(PL) a social psychologist (MB) ) and a chartered counselling
psychologist (LM), we are challenged and heartened by
Joan Roughgarden's call for psychologists to condemn transphobic and
otherwise bigoted research. Like Roughgarden we were troubled
upon reading Bailey's book for its explicit transphobic assumptions
that trans adults are a negative outcome of development and for the
heterosexism, sexism and racism which Roughgarden describes so well.
Trans men, gay and bisexual women are notable by their invisibility
in the text. The use of the authors friends' opinion of bisexuality
as "gay, straight or lying" in the book itself, and now it seems in
advertisements is not perceived as amusing or trivial in our opinion
in view of the slow progress there has been in developing a bisexual
psychology, and the real effects of biphobia in blighting people's
lives. There is very little recognition in mainstream psychology
generally which is further perpetuated by this book, that someone
could be attracted to both sexes or have relationships with both,
with many theorists favouring the general binary construction of
sexuality which does not allow for an 'in between' position; people
are either gay or straight (Ochs, 1996). Generally, many bisexuals
are seen as straight if in a relationship with someone of the
opposite sex, and gay if in a relationship with someone of the same
sex and that experience of having an imposed social identity which
conflicts with a personal identity, and the confusion it engenders
can have commonalities with trans experience. In respect to the "Gaydar"
and discussion of sexual orientation and related behaviour described
in the book, a whole literature of gay and lesbian psychology which
has been painstakingly developed and promoted within mainstream
psychology, appears to have been excluded.
We are particularly concerned that Bailey's work will be seen as
representative of scientific psychological research, both by the
trans community and by other sections of the public. Bailey relies
on a sample size of six - which would not be sufficient for any
experimental or survey research to be published in a peer reviewed
psychology journal. (Indeed, the standard statistical assumptions
upon which quantitative psychological research rests - such as the
central limit theorem - cannot apply to samples of this size). In
this regard Bailey's work is an outlier rather than the norm for
quantitative psychology.
Sometimes psychologists do conduct research with small sample sizes,
and rely on qualitative data rather than quantitative data. Such
research can be particularly useful when conducted among
under-represented and difficult-to-access populations as it can
inform psychologists about a group that it might be difficult to
study statistically. Does Bailey's research then fit the model for
acceptable qualitative psychology? This is questionable. A hallmark
of good qualitative research is reflexivity - an awareness and
description of the way that qualitative data is shaped by the
researcher's own position. Qualitative researchers also frequently
understand their participants as directing the research and
informing its questions. The participants in this research have
provided the case material but cannot be said to be participants in
the sense that is currently considered good practice in
psychological research. There is insufficient discussion of the
limitations of his interviews and too many conclusions are drawn
about the essence of transsexual psychology from casual talk in
bars, occasional anecdotes and the opinions of the author's friends.
The persistent critiques from the trans community (including
Bailey's own participants) support our criticism of this not being
collaborative qualitative research.In spite of the differences
between them, and the debates between quantitative and qualitative
methods in particular, all social scientific methodologies are
designed to ensure that we do not inflate our own opinions
into evidence. In quantitative research this is done by using
methods that limit the effects of the researchers' own perspective
on the data. In qualitative research, it is done by making those
effects part of the data itself. This is not in evidence in the
research reported in "The Man Who Would Be Queen".
As a result the danger that Bailey's expressed anti-trans opinions
might be confused with scientific evidence is particularly high in
this case. Indeed, Bailey repeatedly uses a non-scientific form of
argument, the 'ad hominum', to lend scientific credence to his point
of view. He often cites his own status within scientific communities
(and those of colleagues) but it is important to note that
status within one's own field, (or elsewhere), should count for
nothing in academic debates. For these reasons, the consistent
criticism of Bailey's work from trans scholars, scientists from
other disciplines and activists such as Joan Roughgarden, Jed Bland
and Lynn Conway is particularly welcome to us as psychologists who
are concerned with standards of ethical and scholarly conduct within
our field. Roughgarden is right that there is a history of
transphobic research in psychology. In fact we are surprised that
she describes Bailey's research as 'surprising' as he has been
involved in research on childhood 'gender non-conformity' for some
time (e.g., Bailey & Zucker, 1996). Most of the psychological
research on transsexuality and transgender falls into the abnormal
clinical literature, as did most research on homosexuality up until
the 1970s. Indeed, in contrast to the well-developed fields of
research on heterosexism (and also sexism, ageism, and racism) there
are few studies of transphobia in psychology journals, and no
standardized attitude measure has been published. Clearly there is a
wide open field of trans psychology, premised on the assumption that
trans people are people rather than clinical cases, which is crying
out to be developed. However, it would be wrong to assume that the
methods of psychology are so completely flawed that they render
Bailey's research as paradigmatic.
As psychologists with a special interest in lesbian, gay, bisexual
and transgender psychology and civil rights, we accept the need to
change the way psychology has constructed all of these populations
and to draw from recent works within the field of psychology to
expand our everyday reality about our social worlds. However, we
also recognise the need to become more interdisciplinary and even
multidisciplinary if we really do want to move lesbian, gay,
bisexual, transgender (and dare we say 'queer') studies into the
21st Century. There are growing numbers of critical psychologists
challenging traditional psychological theories and shifting
paradigms. This is particularly evident in the Lesbian and Gay
Psychology Section of the British Psychological Society which
promotes and develops lesbian, gay and bisexual psychological
research and practice not framed from within a heterosexist
framework, as well as including a significant number of
psychologists with a special interest in developing a transgender
psychology which does not pathologise trans people. This will
inevitably take time and patience - despite the need for those
impatient enough to want change, to come forward and become more
visible.
Suggestions for future considerations for transgender psychology
research both for participants to raise prior to being involved in
research and for psychologists to address when designing and seeking
ethical approval for such research:
1. The employment of standard ethical
and scientific procedures.
2. Wide consultation with trans people
and trans activists about hypotheses, research questions, etc, and a
commitment to applying current good practice more commonplace now in
regard to user involvement in more mainstream fields of research to
trans research, particularly when the principal researchers come
from outside the trans community
3. Not to use trans people as 'natural
experiments' to test hypotheses about 'gender' , 'sexual
orientation' etc. in static categorical terms.
4. Inclusion of qualitative and
quantitative data.
5. Development of prejudice research.
6. Recognition that there is an
interface with other minority areas (e.g., psychology of women,
lesbian, gay and bisexual psychology) but not a tokenistic addition
of trans issues to these areas without substantial engagement.
7. Sensitivity to the ways that
research on prejudiced groups will be received and to reflect that
awareness in how the research is disseminated.